Wirksamkeit des computergestützten grapho-phonologischen Trainingsprogramms Lautarium bei Kindern mit Lese-Rechtschreib-Störung
Summary
Effects of the Computer-Based Grapho-Phonological Training Lautarium in Children with Developmental Dyslexia
The effects of the computer-based training program Lautarium on phonological awareness and literacy skills were investigated in 41 third-grade children with developmental dyslexia who attended special dyslexia classes in a primary school in Saxony, Germany. Based on the proven efficacy of phonics-based instruction, Lautarium combines training of phoneme perception and phonological awareness with training of grapheme-phoneme-relationships and reading and spelling of transparent words. In addition, rapid access from written words to meaning is included. The children of the training group (N = 27) worked through the program during school lessons, 5 times per week for 30 minutes, for a period of 7 weeks. During the training period, the controls (N = 14) received traditional remedial reading instruction, 2-3 times per week, in small groups. Children’s performance in phonological awareness, reading, and spelling was assessed at three time points (pretest, immediate posttest, and follow-up after 9 weeks). Pretest scores did not differ between groups. For spelling and subtests of phonological awareness, group comparisons of raw scores at posttest and follow-up including the respective pretest score as covariate confirmed stronger improvements in the training group when compared to the controls. Effect sizes were medium to strong. For reading, improvements did not differ between groups. In both groups, standard scores (T-scores) for reading and spelling increased significantly and substantially across the study period (from pretest to follow-up). The results confirm the efficacy of computer-based training with Lautarium in children with dyslexia, and the efficacy of school-based remedial instruction provided in the dyslexia classes.
References
Adlard, A., Hazan, V. (1998). Speech perception in children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A Human Experimental Psychology, 51, 153-177.
Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 255-259.
Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J. (2004). The nature of phonological awareness: Converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 65-92.
Araújo, S., Faísca, L. (2019). A meta-analytic review of naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23, 349-368.
Araújo, S., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., Faísca, L. (2015). Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 868-883.
Arends, M. (2005). Holta di Polta: Pädagogisch-therapeutisches Übungsmaterial zur Förderung der phonologischen Bewusstheit. Leer: Verl. für Lerntherapeutische Medien.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working Memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Büttner, G., Grube, D., Mähler, C., Hasselhorn, M. (2013). Arbeitsgedächtnisfunktionen von Kindern mit Minderleistungen in der Schriftsprache. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 147-159.
Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Büttner, G., Hasselhorn, M. (2015). Working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading versus spelling: Searching for overlapping and specific cognitive factors. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48, 622-634.
Bus, A. G., IJzendorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness an early reading: A metaanalysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403-414.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., Bryant, P. (2004). Children‘s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31-42.
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., Beni, R. de (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 246-251.
Cassady, J. C., Smith, L. L., Putman, S. M. (2008). Phonological awareness development as a discrete process: Evidence for an integrative model. Reading Psychology, 29, 508-533.
Castles, A., Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to sucess in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77-111.
Castles, A., Rastle, K., Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest : A Journal of the American Psychological Society, 19, 5-51.
Clayton, F. J., West, G., Sears, C., Hulme, C., Lervåg, A. (2019). A longitudinal study of early reading development: Letter-sound knowledge, phoneme awareness and RAN, but not letter-sound integration, predict variations in reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 1-17.
Conrad, N. J., Kennedy, K., Saoud, W., Scallion, L., Hanusiak, L. (2019). Establishing word representations through reading and spelling: Comparing degree of orthographic learning. Journal of Research in Reading, 42, 162-177.
Dilling, H., Freyberger, H. J. (Hrsg.) (2019). Taschenführer zur ICD-10-Klassifikation psychischer Störungen: Mit Glossar und diagnostischen Kriterien sowie Referenztabellen ICD-10 vs. ICD-9 und ICD-10 vs. DSM-IV-TR (9., aktual. Aufl. unter Berücksichtigung der Änderungen gemäss ICD-10-GM (German Modification) 2019). Bern: Hogrefe.
Dummer-Smoch, L., Hackethal, R. (2016). Kieler Leseaufbau (9. Auflage). Kiel: Veris-Verl.
Ecalle, J., Magnan, A., Bouchafa, H., Gombert, J. E. (2009). Computer-based training with ortho-phonological units in dyslexic children: New investigations. Dyslexia (Chichester, England), 15, 218-238.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the national reading panel`s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly.
Elbro, C., Jensen, M. N. (2005). Quality of phonological representations, verbal learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375-384.
Ennemoser, M., Marx, P., Weber, J., Schneider, W. (2012). Spezifische Vorläuferfertigkeiten der Lesegeschwindigkeit, des Leseverständnisses und des Rechtschreibens. Zeitschrift Für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 44, 53-67.
Esser, G., Wyschkon, A., Schmidt, M. H. (2002). Was wird aus Achtjährigen mit einer Lese- und Rechtschreibstörung. Zeitschrift Für Klinische Psychologie Und Psychotherapie, 31, 235-242.
Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Balke-Melcher, C., Schmidt, C., . . . Hasselhorn, M. (2013). Prävalenz von Lernschwächen und Lernstörungen: Zur Bedeutung der Diagnosekriterien. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 65-76.
Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Mähler, C., Hasselhorn, M. (2010). Zeigen Kinder mit schulischen Minderleistungen sozio-emotionale Auffälligkeiten? Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 42, 201-210.
Fischer, M. Y., Pfost, M. (2015). Wie effektiv sind Maßnahmen zur Förderung der phonologischen Bewusstheit? Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 47, 35-51.
Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49-65.
Galuschka, K., Görgen, R., Kalmar, J., Haberstroh, S., Schmalz, X., Schulte-Körne, G. (2019). Effectiveness of spelling treatment approaches for learners with dyslexia – A meta-analysis and systematic review.
Galuschka, K., Ise, E., Krick, K., Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PloS One, 9, e89900
González-Valenzuela, M.-J. (2017). Current perspectives on prevention of reading and writing learning disabilities. In C. S. Ryan (Hrsg.), Learning disabilities – An international perspective. InTech.
Groth, K., Lachmann, T., Riecker, A., Muthmann, I., Steinbrink, C. (2011). Developmental dyslexics show deficits in the processing of temporal auditory information in German vowel length discrimination. Reading and Writing, 24,285-303.
Hasselhorn, M., Schuchardt, K. (2006). Lernstörungen. Eine kritische Skizze zur Epidemiologie. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 15, 208-215.
Huettig, F., Lachmann, T., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M. (2018). Distinguishing cause from effect – many deficits associated with developmental dyslexia may be a consequence of reduced and suboptimal reading experience. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33, 333-350.
Ise, E., Engel, R. R., Schulte-Körne, G. (2012). Was hilft bei der Lese-Rechtschreibstörung? Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21, 122-136.
Ke, F., Abras, T. (2013). Games for engaged learning of middle school children with special learning needs. British Journal of Educational Technology, 44, 225-242.
Kirby, J. R., Georgiou, G. K., Martinussen, R., Parrila, R., Bowers, P. G., Landerl, K. (2010). Naming speed and reading: From prediction to instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 341-362.
Klatte, M., Bergström, K., Konerding, M., Lachmann, T. (in Vorbereitung). Kaiserslauterer Gruppentest zur Lautbewusstheit (KaLaube). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Klatte, M., Bergström, K., Steinbrink, C., Konerding, M., Lachmann, T. (2018). Effects of the computer-based training program Lautarium on phonological awareness and reading and spelling abilities in German second-graders. In T. Lachmann, T. Weis (Hrsg.), Reading and Dyslexia: From basic functions to higher order cognition (S. 323-339). Cham: Springer.
Klatte, M., Steinbrink, C., Bergström, K., Lachmann, T. (2013). Phonologische Verarbeitung bei Grundschulkindern mit schwacher Lesefähigkeit. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 199-215.
Klatte, M., Steinbrink, C., Bergström, K., Lachmann, T. (2016). Lautarium – Ein computerbasiertes Trainingsprogramm für Grundschulkinder mit Lese-Rechtschreibschwierigkeiten. In M. Hasselhorn, W. Schneider (Hrsg.), Tests und Trends: Neue Folge Band 14. Förderprogramme für Vor- und Grundschule (S. 115-141). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Klatte, M., Steinbrink, C., Bergström, K., Lachmann, T. (2017). Lautarium: Ein computerbasiertes Trainingsprogramm für Grundschulkinder mit Lese-Rechtschreibschwierigkeiten: Manual. Hogrefe Förderprogramme. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Klatte, M., Steinbrink, C., Prölß, A., Estner, B., Christmann, C. A., Lachmann, T. (2014). Effekte des computerbasierten Trainingsprogramms „Lautarium“ auf die phonologische Verarbeitung und die Lese-Rechtschreibleistungen bei Grundschulkindern. In G. Schulte-Körne (Hrsg.), Legasthenie und Dyskalkulie (S. 127-144). Bochum: Winkler.
Klauer, K. (1989). Denktraining für Kinder I. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Kohn, J., Wyschkon, A., Ballaschk, K., Ihle, W., Esser, G. (2013). Verlauf von umschriebenen Entwicklungsstörungen: Eine 30-Monats-Follow-up-Studie. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 77-89.
Kohn, J., Wyschkon, A., Esser, G. (2013). Psychische Auffälligkeiten bei umschriebenen Entwicklungsstörungen: Gibt es Unterschiede zwischen Lese-Rechtschreib- und Rechenstörungen? Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 7-20.
Konerding, M., Bergström, K., Lachmann, T., Klatte, M. (2020). Effects of computerized grapho-phonological training on literacy acquisition and vocabulary knowledge in children with an immigrant background learning German as L2. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 28, 367-383.
Lachmann, T. (2002). Reading disability as a deficit in functional coordination and information integration. In E. Witruk, A. D. Friederici, T. Lachmann (Hrsg.), Basic functions of language, reading and reading disability, 165-198. Boston: Kluwer/Springer.
Lachmann, T. (2018). Reading and Dyslexia: The functional coordination framework. In T. Lachmann, T. Weis (Hrsg.), Reading and Dyslexia: From basic functions to higher order cognition. (S. 271-296). Cham: Springer.
Landerl, K. (2003). Categorization of vowel length in German poor spellers: An orthographically relevant phonological distinction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 523-538.
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., de Jong, P. F., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., . . . Georgiou, G. K. (2019). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23, 220-234.
Landerl, K., Moll, K. (2010). Comorbidity of learning disorders: Prevalence and familial transmission. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 287-294.
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150-161.
Lenhard, W., Schneider, W. (2006). Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst- bis Sechstklässler: (ELFE 1 - 6). Deutsche Schultests. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Hämäläinen, J., Torppa, M., Ronimus, M. (2015). Dyslexia-early identification and prevention: Highlights from the Jyväskylä longitudinal study of dyslexia. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2, 330-338.
Macaruso, P., Rodman, A. (2011). Efficacy of computer-assisted instruction for the development of early literacy skills in young children. Reading Psychology, 32, 172-196.
Mähler, C., Joerns, C., Schuchardt, K. (2019). Training working memory of children with and without dyslexia. Children (Basel, Switzerland), 6.
Mähler, C., Jörns, C., Radtke, E., Schuchardt, K. (2015). Chancen und Grenzen eines Trainings des Arbeitsgedächtnisses bei Kindern mit und ohne Lese-/Rechtschreibschwierigkeiten. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft, 18, 453-471.
May, P. (2012). Hamburger Schreib-Probe 3 (HSP 3): Hinweise zur Durchführung und Auswertung. Stuttgart: VPM; Klett.
McArthur, G., Ellis, D., Atkinson, C. M., Coltheart, M. (2008). Auditory processing deficits in children with reading and language impairments: Can they (and should they) be treated? Cognition, 107, 946-977.
McArthur, G., Eve, P. M., Jones, K., Banales, E., Kohnen, S., Anandakumar, T., . . . Castles, A. (2012). Phonics training for English-speaking poor readers. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 12, CD009115.
McArthur, G., Sheehan, Y., Badcock, N. A., Francis, D. A., Wang, H. C., Kohnen, S., . . . Castles, A. (2018). Phonics training for English-speaking poor readers. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 11, CD009115.
McBride-Chang, C. (1995). Phonological processing, speech perception, and reading disability: An integrative review. Educational Psychologist, 30, 109-121.
Melby-Lervåg, M., Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270-279.
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322-352.
Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responses. Brain and Language, 72, 238-245.
Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J., Bruder, J., Kunze, S., Neuhoff, N., . . . Landerl, K. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 65-77.
O‘Connell, N. S., Dai, L., Jiang, Y., Speiser, J. L., Ward, R., Wei, W., . . . Gebregziabher, M. (2017). Methods for analysis of pre-post data in clinical research: A comparison of five common methods. Journal of Biometrics & Biostatistics, 8, 1-8.
Parrila, R., Dudley, D., Song, S., Georgiou, G. K. (2020). A meta-analysis of reading-level match dyslexia studies in consistent alphabetic orthographies. Annals of Dyslexia, 70, 1-26.
Pennington, B. F., Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283-306.
Pfost, M. (2015). Children’s phonological awareness as a predictor of reading and spelling. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 47, 123-138.
Rausch, J. R., Maxwell, S. E., Kelley, K. (2003). Analytic methods for questions pertaining to a randomized pretest, posttest, follow-up design. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 467-486.
Raven, H. J., Raven, J., Court, J. H. (2002). Ravens progressive matrices and vocalubary scales. Coulered Progressive Matrices (CPM). Deutsche Bearbeitung und Normierung von Stephan Bulheller und Hartmut Häcker. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services.
Ronimus, M., Eklund, K., Pesu, L., Lyytinen, H. (2019). Supporting struggling readers with digital game-based learning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 67, 639-663.
Schaadt, G., Männel, C., van der Meer, E., Pannekamp, A., Oberecker, R., Friederici, A. D. (2015). Present and past: Can writing abilities in school children be associated with their auditory discrimination capacities in infancy? Research in Developmental Disabilities, 47, 318-333.
Schnitzler, C. D. (2008). Phonologische Bewusstheit und Schriftspracherwerb. Forum Logopädie. Stuttgart, New York: Thieme.
Schulte-Körne, G., Bruder, J. (2010). Clinical neurophysiology of visual and auditory processing in dyslexia: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 121, 1794-1809.
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European ortho-graphies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174.
Shahar-Yames, D., Share, D. L. (2008). Spelling as a self-teaching mechanism in orthographic learning. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 22-39.
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
Snowling, M. J., Melby-Lervåg, M. (2016). Oral language deficits in familial dyslexia: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 498-545.
Steinbrink, C., Klatte, M., Lachmann, T. (2014). Phonological, temporal and spectral processing in vowel length discrimination is impaired in German primary school children with developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 3034-3045.
Steinbrink, C., Lachmann, T. (2014). Lese-Rechtschreibstörung: Grundlagen, Diagnostik, Intervention. Berlin: Springer VS.
Strehlow, U., Haffner, J., Bischof, J., Gratzka, V., Parzer, P., Resch, F. (2006). Does successful training of temporal processing of sound and phoneme stimuli improve reading and spelling? European Psychologist, 15, 19-29.
Suggate, S. P. (2010). Why what we teach depends on when: Grade and reading intervention modality moderate effect size. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1556-1579.
Swan, D., Goswami, U. (1997). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 18-41.
Thomé, G. (2000). Linguistische und psycholinguistische Grundlagen der Orthografie: Die Schrift und das Schreibenlernen. In R. Valentin (Hrsg.), Rechtschreiben lernen in den Klassen 1-6. Grundlagen und didaktische Hilfen (S. 12-16). Frankfurt am Main: Grundschulverband.
Thomson, J. M., Leong, V., Goswami, U. (2013). Auditory processing interventions and developmental dyslexia: a comparison of phonemic and rhythmic approaches. Reading and Writing, 26, 139-161.
Van der Stappen, C., van Reybroeck, M. (2018). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming are independent phonological competencies with specific impacts on word reading and spelling: An intervention study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, article 320, 1-16.
Vandermosten, M., Correia, J., Vanderauwera, J., Wouters, J., Ghesquière, P., Bonte, M. (2020). Brain activity patterns of phonemic representations are atypical in beginning readers with family risk for dyslexia. Developmental Science, 23, e12857.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., . . . Garon, T. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H. (2002). Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 272-277.
Wouters, P., van Nimwegen, C., van Oostendorp, H., van der Spek, E. D. (2013). A meta-analysis of the cognitive and motivational effects of serious games. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 249-265.
Wyschkon, A., Schulz, F., Gallit, F. S., Poltz, N., Kohn, J., Moraske, S., . . . Esser, G. (2018). 5-Jahres-Verlauf der LRS: Stabilität, Geschlechtseffekte, Schriftsprachniveau und Schulerfolg. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 46, 107-122.
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., Saine, N., Lyytinen, H., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551-559.
Ziegler, J. C., Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29.
Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Lorenzi, C. (2009). Speech-perception-in-noise-deficits in dyslexia. Developmental Science, 12, 731-745.
Zoubrinetzky, R., Collet, G., Nguyen-Morel, M.-A., Valdois, S., Serniclaes, W. (2019). Remediation of allophonic perception and visual attention span in developmental dyslexia: A joint assay. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1502.